Skip to main content

Effect of lunch with different calorie and nutrient balances on dinner-induced postprandial glucose variability

Abstract

Aim

This study aimed to examine the effect of lunches with different caloric contents (Study 1) and nutrient balances (Study 2) on dinner-induced postprandial glucose fluctuation.

Methods

Energy trial (Study 1): Thirteen healthy young participants (n = 10 men, n = 3 women) were investigated to determine the effects of different caloric intakes at lunch on glucose level variability. The study was comprised of four trials (no lunch, low lunch, standard lunch, and high-energy lunch). Energy balance trial (Study 2): Fourteen healthy young adults (n = 8 men, n = 6 women) were investigated to determine the effect of different nutrient balances during lunch on glucose level variability. The study consisted of four trials (standard, protein-rich, fat-rich, and carbohydrate-rich). In studies 1 and 2, each trial was spaced at least 24 full hours apart, and breakfast and dinner were tested as meals. The mealtimes for each trial were then aligned. Continuous glucose monitoring was used to assess the blood glucose fluctuations.

Results

Study 1: The no-lunch (95% CI 95.5–149.7) and low-energy lunch (95% CI 90.8–143.1) trials had significantly higher values in the incremental area under the curve (iAUC) of postprandial blood glucose at dinner compared to the standard (95% CI 55.4–90.0) and high-energy lunch (95% CI 29.3–54.6) trials (P = 0.006, P = 0.001 vs. none), (P = 0.004, P = 0.001 vs. low-energy trial). Study 2: A significantly higher postprandial blood glucose iAUC for dinner was found in the fat-rich trial (95% CI 58.5–114.0) than that in the protein-rich (95% CI 25.6–63.9) and standard (95% CI 25.6–112.4) trials, (P = 0.006, P = 0.035 vs. fat-rich trial).

Conclusions

Our findings indicate that skipping lunch and low-calorie or high-lipid intake increased postprandial blood glucose levels after dinner.

Introduction

Previous studies on meal patterns in people with obesity have focused primarily on the association between meal size [1], timing [2], and frequency [3]. Recently, in a systematic review and network meta-analysis that examined the relationship between meal frequency and obesity [3], one meal per day was ranked as the best frequency for reducing body weight, followed by two meals per day, but not by three meals per day, compared with > 8 meals per day. In contrast, two meals per day performed best for the reduction of waist size compared with six meals per day [3]. This review suggests that 3 meals per day may not be the best way to control body weight. Several studies have suggested that habitual breakfast skipping is related to health problems such as the risk of obesity [4], cardiovascular diseases [5], and cognitive function [6]. A survey of feeding patterns in Japan reported that many people have a meal ratio (10 for whole day’s energy intake) of 2.5 for breakfast; 3, lunch; and, 4.5, dinner [7]. At dinner, obesity is positively related to not only the energy quantity of meals but also the eating time of meals. These studies suggest a relatively large energy quantity of breakfast and a relatively small energy quantity of dinner for health promotion. However, few reports have demonstrated the role of lunch in health promotion.

Little is known about the relationship between employees’ meal-skipping patterns and workplace dietary choices and health [8]. Employees who skipped breakfast on > 3 days/week had lower healthy purchase index (HPI) compared with those who never skipped breakfast [8]. In addition, skipping lunch on > 3 days/week and dinner > 1 day/week were associated with significantly lower HPC than never skipping lunch. This study suggests that not skipping lunch may help with the purchase of a healthier meal for lunch. During the COVID-19 pandemic lockdowns, practices of healthy eating, skipping lunch, and more frequent physical activity were significantly associated with weight loss [9]. Although breakfast skipping is related to obesity, this study suggests the possibility of skipping lunch for weight loss. However, there are no detailed reports on the effect of lunch skipping and/or unhealthy lunch on postprandial glucose levels at dinner. In Iran in 2015, a cross-sectional nationwide study (14,286 students aged 7–18) revealed that the frequencies of breakfast, lunch, and dinner skipping were 13.85, 6.8%, and 7.5%, respectively [10]. Thus, the percentage of lunch skipping is not near 0%, but approximately half as that of breakfast skipping, and, lunch skipping should be taken into consideration in addition to breakfast skipping.

Several studies have demonstrated that water-soluble dietary fiber consumption [11,12,13], low-glycemic index meal consumption [14, 15], and protein-rich meal consumption [16, 17] can influence the postprandial glycemic response at the next meal. This effect has led to the glucose levels following the second meal being attenuated. When fiber-rich food is consumed at breakfast as a first meal, the following second meal, such as lunch, it attenuates the increase in glucose levels compared with the usual food [18]. In a recent cross-over experiment in humans, we demonstrated that the consumption of 5 g dry powder (approximately 50% inulin) of artichokes at breakfast time attenuated glucose increase after lunch and dinner compared with no artichoke intake [19]. Furthermore, snacks after lunch have also been reported to affect glucose level changes at dinner time. Fiber-rich snacks attenuate tissue glucose increase at dinner time in healthy individuals [20, 21]. Moreover, consuming snacks in the late afternoon attenuated the tissue glucose level at dinner in patients with type 2 diabetes [22]. Thus, lunch may affect the glucose level change at dinner as a second-meal effect.

Several previous studies have revealed that the increase in blood glucose is higher during lunch and/or dinner than during breakfast [23, 24]. In addition, in our recent crossover trial in which participants ate the same meals at breakfast or dinner times on different days, the postprandial tissue glucose increase was higher at the dinner meal than at the breakfast meal, suggesting circadian clock control of glucose level through insulin release and/or insulin receptor sensitivity [25]. Tachyphagia (a meal lasting < 15 min) rapidly increases blood glucose levels and promotes weight gain. Recently, it was reported that eating while standing and eating fast food at lunchtime were positively associated with tachyphagia [26]. Previous studies have reported that meal skipping can affect postprandial blood glucose levels in the next meal [27,28,29,30]. Thus, the meal composition of lunch may affect the postprandial glucose increase through tachyphagia.

We aimed to investigate the amount of energy and components of lunch for controlling glucose levels at dinner. Therefore, in the first part of the current study, we examined whether the amount of energy change from 0 kcal to approximately 1000 kcal at lunch as the first meal affected postprandial glucose levels after dinner as the second meal. Second, we examined whether the standard amount of energy of lunch with unbalanced nutrients (protein-rich, fat-rich, or carbohydrate-rich) affected postprandial glucose levels after dinner.

Materials and methods

Participant

This study included healthy young adults (N = 32; 21 men and 11 women) from Tokyo (Japan). The study was conducted between April 2021 and February 2022, and the inclusion criteria were as follows: (1) not using glucose/insulin-lowering or related medications; (2) not having doctor-diagnosed diabetes; and (3) not taking antidiabetic supplements. This study was conducted in accordance with the Helsinki Declaration and was approved by the Ethics Committee for Humans at Waseda University (approval number: 2020-371). The human trial of the current study is registered at UMIN (www.umin.ac.jp/ctr/ number: UMIN000043287). Informed consent was obtained from all the participants after the experiment was described in detail.

Participants were excluded from the study for the following reasons: non-compliance with the protocol (n = 1) and failure of a glucose-monitoring sensor (n = 4). Consequently, twenty-seven participants were included in the final analysis (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Consolidated standards of reporting trials flow diagram. The energy trial group (A) and the balance trial group (B)

Study design

A randomized crossover design was used in this study, with each participant consuming four different lunches. The participants were randomly assigned to the following two groups: the energy trial group (n = 13, n = 10 men and n = 3 women; Table 1A) and the balance trial group (n = 14, n = 8 men and n = 6 women; Table 1B). For the energy trial, the protein, fat, and carbohydrate (PFC) balance at lunch was similar, but blood glucose fluctuations were examined when caloric intake was changed. Four types of lunches were prepared in the energy trial: (1) no, (2) low, (3) standard, and (4) high-energy lunch trials. On the other hand, for the balance trial, the caloric intakes at lunch were similar; however, the PFC balance was changed and subsequent blood glucose level fluctuations were examined. Four types of lunches were prepared in the nutritional balance trial: (1) standard, (2) protein-rich, (3) fat-rich, and (4) carbohydrate-rich.

Table 1 Characteristics of participants

The trial for each group was structured into two weeks, with each trial spaced at least 24 full hours apart. As one of the purposes of the study was to examine blood glucose fluctuations in daily life, each participant was asked to follow his or her usual meal time and was instructed to align the times of breakfast, lunch, and dinner intake in each trial. Only designated meals were allowed in each trial. The diet was prepared based on the results of the National Health and Nutrition Survey in Japan [31]. Strenuous exercise was prohibited during the study period.

Test meals

Test meals for breakfast and dinner

All trial breakfasts and dinners were provided as test meals. Fruit granola 60 g (Calbee, Inc., Tokyo, Japan) and Milk 200 mL (Morinaga & Co., Ltd., Tokyo, Japan) were served for breakfast. The energy intake was 399.8 kcal (Table 2A). For dinner, a Sukiyaki bento (Tokatsu Foods Co., Ltd., Kanagawa, Japan), Rice 200 g (Hagoromo Foods Co., Shizuoka, Japan), Hot spring egg 52 g (Lawson, Inc., Tokyo, Japan), and Miso soup (Marukome Co., Ltd., Nagano, Japan) were used. Miso soup was consumed by dissolving 18 g miso in 160 mL hot water for men or 36 g miso in 320 mL for women. The energy intake differed between men and women; it was 913.3 kcal for men and 655.5 kcal for women (Table 2B, C).

Table 2 Test meals for each trial

Study meals for energy trial

The nutritional balance of lunch for each trial was set at P: 18.2%, F: 36.4%, and C: 45.4%. For the low-energy lunch trial, Calorie Mate Jelly 108 g for men or 86 g for women (Otsuka Pharmaceutical Co., Ltd., Tokyo, Japan) and Protein Bar Wafer Vanilla 19 g (Morinaga & Co., Ltd., Tokyo, Japan) for men or 15 g for women were served for lunch. For the standard-energy lunch trial, Calorie Mate Jelly 376 g for men or 286 g for women and Protein Bar Wafer Vanilla 65 g for men or 49 g for women were served for lunch. For the high-energy lunch trial, Calorie Mate Jelly 645 g for men or 486 g for women and Protein Bar Wafer Vanilla 111 g for men or 84 g for women were served for lunch. Details of the meals for each trial are shown in Table 2D, E.

Study meals for balance trial

For the standard trial, Calorie mate jelly 376 g for men or 286 g for women (Otsuka Pharmaceutical Co., Ltd., Tokyo, Japan) and a Protein bar 65 g for men or 49 g for women (Morinaga & Co., Ltd., Tokyo, Japan) were served for lunch. In the protein-rich trial, chicken salad 3 pieces for men 2 pieces for women (Lawson, Inc., Tokyo, Japan), Savas whey protein powder 21 g (Meiji Co., Ltd., Tokyo, Japan), Milk 200 mL (Morinaga & Co., Ltd., Tokyo, Japan), and Calorie mate bars 20 g for men or 6 g for women (Otsuka Pharmaceutical Co., Ltd., Tokyo, Japan) were served for lunch. In the fat-rich trial, Cheese 90 g for men or 36 g for women (Life Co., Tokyo, Japan), Peanuts 30 g (Life Co., Tokyo), and Calorie Mate bars 40 g (Otsuka Pharmaceutical Co., Ltd., Tokyo, Japan) were served for lunch. In the carbohydrate-rich trial, Papatto rice 200 g, (Hagoromo Foods Co., Shizuoka, Japan), Dried seasoning powder 5 g (Marumiya Foods Industries Co., Ltd., Tokyo, Japan), and Calorie mate Jelly 430 g for men or 280 g for women (Otsuka Pharmaceutical Co., Ltd., Tokyo, Japan) were served for lunch. The caloric intake and energy balance of the test meal varied between the male and female participants. The details of the meals for each trial are shown in Tables 2F and G.

Measurements

Anthropometry and chronotype

Anthropometric variables were measured before the study. Height was measured to the nearest 0.1 cm with a portable Seca 213 height meter (As One Corporation, Osaka, Japan). Body weight was measured to the nearest 0.1 kg using InBody 270 (InBody Japan Corporation, Tokyo, Japan). Body mass index (BMI) was also calculated using InBody. Muscle mass, fat mass, and body fat percentage were measured by the bioelectrical impedance analysis (BIA) method using InBody 270 (InBody Japan Corporation, Tokyo, Japan).

The chronotype was assessed using the Horne–Ostberg Morningness–Eveningness Questionnaire (MEQ) [31], which consists of 19 questions related to preferred sleep time and daily performance. The sum yielded scores ranging from 16 to 86. Based on their scores, the participants were divided into three chronotype groups: morningness (score 59–86), intermediate (score 42–58), and eveningness (score 16–41).

Dietary intake

The participants’ energy intakes were assessed using a food frequency questionnaire (FFQ) [32]. The FFQ estimated the actual food intake by surveying the frequency and quantity of food intake using a questionnaire. The frequency and intake of breakfast, lunch, and dinner were surveyed, and the energy and nutrient intakes of the usual meals were calculated.

Physical activity levels

All the participants were asked to wear a triaxial accelerometer (Active style Pro HJA-750C; Omron Corp., Kyoto, Japan) during the study period. The participants were instructed to wear the activity meter at all times, except while bathing and sleeping. The data were valid only if the accelerometer was worn for at least 10 h (600 min) per day. Step count and moderate-to-vigorous physical activity (MVPA) were used for the assessment.

Glucose levels

On the first day of the study, a continuous glucose-monitoring (CGM) device, FreeStyle Libre Pro (Abbott Japan LLC, Tokyo, Japan), was placed on the participant's upper arm. After the study, the device was retrieved, and the data were read by a dedicated device. This device can measure glucose levels in the interstitial fluid every 15 min for 14 consecutive days. An observation period of at least half a day was established to stabilize glucose monitoring. For all trials, glycemic variability and peak glucose levels were calculated at a maximum of 4 h after lunch and dinner intake. The area under the curve (AUC) and incremental area under the curve (iAUC) were calculated from lunch and dinner consumption to 120, 180, and 240 min.

Statistical analysis

All the data are presented as the mean (standard deviation). First, the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test was used to test for normality before statistical processing. Correlation analysis using the Pearson product-moment correlation coefficient was used to examine the relationship between the amount of physical activity and the standard trial for both the trials. One-way repeated-measures ANOVA was used to compare the AUC and iAUC after lunch and dinner for each trial. The Friedman test was used when normality was not observed. Correlation analysis with Pearson product-moment correlation coefficient was used to examine the relationship between the usual lunch intake or percentage of energy intake and after-dinner blood glucose levels in each trial. The Spearman test was used to examine the relationship between the differences in meal intake times for each trial in the energy trial and the after-dinner iAUC, as normality was not observed. On the other hand, Pearson’s product-moment correlation coefficient was used to examine the relationship between the difference in meal intake times and after-dinner iAUC for each trial in the balance trial, because normality was observed. A one-way ANOVA was also used to test the peak postprandial blood glucose values for each trial. The Friedman test was used when normality was not observed. A t-test was used to test comparisons of characteristics by mean starvation time. In addition, a two-way ANOVA was used to compare postprandial blood glucose variability between trials. All the data were analyzed using IBM SPSS Statistics statistical analysis software for Windows (SPSS Japan Inc., Tokyo, Japan). Statistical significance was set at P < 0.05.

Results

Relationship between the physical activity levels and AUC

In the energy trial, the step counts and MVPA per day during the study period were 9545.6 (4433.5) steps (95% CI 6603.4–1248.7) and 91.5 (39.1) min (95% CI 65.6–117.5). In the balance trial, the step counts and MVPA per day during the study period were 9905.8 (3546.6) steps (95% CI 7552.2–28.0) and 100.6 (28.1) min (95% CI 80.9–26.6–120.3) (Additional file 3: Fig. S3E, F). The correlation between physical activity level and the AUC of postprandial glucose level after dinner was analyzed during the study period. Step counts and MVPA were used as indices of physical activity level. In the energy trial, there was no significant correlation between step counts or MVPA during the study period and the after-dinner AUC in the standard lunch trial (Additional file 1: Fig. S1A, B). In the balance trial, there was no significant correlation between step counts or MVPA during the study period and the after-dinner AUC in the standard trial (Additional file 1: Fig. S1C, D).

Comparison of the glucose levels in the energy trial

The 24-h blood glucose excursion in the energy trial is shown in Additional file 2: Fig. S2A. The monitoring of glucose levels for 4 h after lunch showed an increase in postprandial blood glucose levels in the standard- and high-energy lunch trials (Fig. 2B). Significant differences between energy trials after lunch at each 15-min time interval were examined (Additional file 2: Supplemental Fig. S2C). The AUC for 4 h after lunch was 339.6 (39.4) (95% CI 315.8–363.4) in the no-energy lunch trial, 354.7 (37.5) (95% CI 332.0–377.3) in the low-energy lunch trial, 403.7 (39.7) (95% CI 379.7–427.7) in the standard-energy lunch trial and 418.4 (29.9) (95% CI 400.3–436.5) in high-energy lunch trial. The AUC for 4 h after lunch showed no significant difference between the no and low-energy lunch trials (P = 0.107) and between the standard- and high-energy lunch trials (P = 0.601, 95%CI = 16.2) (Fig. 2C). The peak for 4 h after lunch was 89.9 (12.1) (95% CI 82.6–97.3) in the no-energy lunch trial, 108.5 (11.0) (95% CI 101.9–115.2) in the low-energy lunch trial, 131.0 (18.3) (95% CI 119.9–142.1) in the standard-energy lunch trial and 126.5 (12.6) (95% CI 118.9–134.1) in high-energy lunch trial. The peak for 4 h after lunch showed a significant decrease in no-energy lunch trial compared with the low-, standard- and high-energy lunch trial (P = 0.004, P = 0.001, P = 0.001 vs no-energy lunch trial). The peak for 4 h after lunch showed a significant decrease in the low-energy lunch trial compared with the standard- and high-energy lunch trial (P = 0.001, P = 0.002 vs low-energy lunch trial). (Fig. 2D). Monitoring of glucose levels for 4 h after dinner showed an increase in postprandial blood glucose levels in the no- and low-energy lunch trials (Fig. 2E). Significant differences between energy trials after dinner at each 15-min time interval were examined (Additional file 2: Fig. S2D). The iAUC for 2 h after dinner was 122.6 (59.1) (95% CI 95.5–149.7) in the no-energy lunch trial, 117.0 (55.4) (95% CI 90.8–143.1) in the low-energy lunch trial, 72.7 (44.0) (95% CI 55.4–90.0) in the standard-energy lunch trial and 42.0 (34.9) (95% CI 29.3–54.6) in high-energy lunch trial. On examining the iAUC for 2 h after dinner, the no and low-energy lunch trials showed an increase in iAUC after dinner compared with the standard- and high-energy lunch trials (P = 0.006, P = 0.001 vs none), (P = 0.004, P = 0.001 vs low-energy trial) (Fig. 2F). The AUC for 3 h after dinner was 403.2 (46.5) (95% CI 375.1–431.3) in the no-energy lunch trial, 405.2 (49.2) (95% CI 375.4–434.9) in the low-energy lunch trial, 366.5 (39.0) (95% CI 342.9–390.0) in the standard-energy lunch trial and 332.4 (35.2) (95% CI 311.2–353.7) in high-energy lunch trial. On examining the AUC for 3 h after dinner, the standard-energy lunch trial had a significantly lower value than the no- and low-energy lunch trials (P = 0.046, P = 0.019 vs standard-energy lunch trial), and the high-energy lunch trial had a significantly lower value than the no-, low-, and standard-energy lunch trials (P = 0.002, P = 0.001 vs high-energy lunch trial) (Fig. 2G). The sum of the AUC for 4 h after lunch and dinner was 843.0 (54.8) (95% CI 808.5–877.4) in the no-energy lunch trial, 854.5 (74.7) (95% CI 807.5–901.5) in the low-energy lunch trial, 867.5 (61.1) (95% CI 829.1–906.0) in the standard-energy lunch trial and 847.8 (61.9) (95% CI 808.9–886.8) in high-energy lunch trial. On comparing the sum of the AUC for 4 h after lunch and dinner, the standard-energy lunch trial had a significantly higher value than the no-lunch trial (Fig. 2H). The peak glucose levels after dinner were 183.1 (26.4) (95% CI 167.1–199.0) in the no-energy lunch trial, 181.5 (22.1) (95% CI 168.1–194.8) in the low-energy lunch trial, 162.2 (23.3) (95% CI 148.1–176.2) in the standard-energy lunch trial, and 134.9 (21.9) (95% CI 127.2–148.1) in the high-energy lunch trial. The peak glucose levels after dinner in the no-, low-, and standard-energy lunch trials were significantly higher than that in the high-energy lunch trial (P = 0.000, P = 0.000, P = 0.006, vs high-energy lunch trial) (Fig. 2I).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Postprandial blood glucose levels for lunch and dinner in the energy trial. All data are presented as the mean (standard deviation). Study protocol for the energy trial (A). Glucose concentration (B), area under the curve (AUC) (C), and peak blood glucose levels (D) for 4-h after lunch. Glucose concentration (E) for 4 h after dinner. Incremental AUC (iAUC) (F) and AUC (G) at 2 and 3 h after dinner. Sum of the AUC for 4 h after lunch and dinner for each trial in the energy trial (H). Peak blood glucose levels at 4 h after dinner (I). Figure 2C and I used One-way ANOVA. D, F, G, and H used the Friedman test.

Comparison of the glucose levels in the balance trial

The 24-h blood glucose excursion in the balance trial is shown in Additional file 2: Fig. S2B. In the balance trial, monitoring glucose levels for 4 h after lunch showed an increase in postprandial blood glucose levels in the carbohydrate-rich trial (Fig. 3B). Significant differences between balance trials after lunch at each 15-min time interval were examined (Additional file 2: Fig. S2E). The AUC for 4 h after lunch was 400.9 (35.6) (95% CI 380.3–421.5) in the standard trial, 367.5 (50.8) (95% CI 338.1–396.8) in the protein-rich trial, 361.2 (33.4) (95% CI 341.9–380.4) in the fat-rich trial and 473.0 (61.9) (95% CI 437.2–508.7) in the carbohydrate-rich trial. The AUC for 4 h after lunch showed a significant difference between the standard and other trials and between the carbohydrate-rich and other trials (P = 0.019 vs protein-rich trial, P = 0.006 vs fat-rich trial) (P = 0.006 vs standard trial, P = 0.004 vs protein-rich trial, P = 0.001 vs fat-rich trial) (Fig. 3C). The peak for 4 h after lunch was 124.9 (14.9) (95% CI 116.3–133.6) in the standard trial, 102.6 (25.1) (95% CI 88.1–117.1) in the protein-rich trial, 104.7 (14.3) (95% CI 96.5–113.0) in the fat-rich trial, and 146.7 (25.9) (95% CI 131.8–161.7) in the carbohydrate-rich trial. The peak for 4 h after lunch showed a significant increase in the carbohydrate-rich trial compared with the standard, protein-rich and fat-rich trial (P = 0.013, P = 0.004, P = 0.001 vs carbohydrate-rich trial). The peak for 4 h after lunch showed a significant increase in the standard trial compared with the protein-rich and fat-rich trial (P = 0.023, P = 0.002 vs standard trial) (Fig. 3D). Monitoring glucose levels for 4 h after dinner showed an increase in postprandial blood glucose levels in the fat-rich trial (Fig. 3E). Significant differences between balance trials after dinner at each 15-min time interval were examined (Additional file 2: Fig. S2F). The iAUC for 2 h after dinner was 57.5 (37.4) (95% CI 25.6–112.4) in the standard trial, 49.4 (28.0) (95% CI 25.6–63.9) in the protein-rich trial, 92.4 (41.4) (95% CI 58.5–114.0) in the fat-rich trial and 60.7 (41.8) (95% CI 29.1–84.9) in the carbohydrate-rich trial. On examining the iAUC for 2 h after dinner, there was a significantly higher rate in the fat-rich trials than those in the standard and protein-rich trials (P = 0.034, P = 0.022 vs fat-rich trials) (Fig. 3F). The AUC for 3 h after dinner was 348.4 (54.2) (95% CI 317.1–379.7) in the standard trial, 333.4 (39.8) (95% CI 310.4–356.4) in the protein-rich trial, 376.6 (62.6) (95% CI 340.4–412.7) in the fat-rich trial and 341.3 (57.3) (95% CI 308.2–374.4) in the carbohydrate-rich trial. A significantly higher AUC for 3 h after dinner was observed in the fat-rich trial than that in the protein-rich trial (P = 0.050 vs protein-rich trial) (Fig. 3G). The sum of the AUC for 4 h after lunch and dinner was 846.5 (80.9) (95% CI 799.8–893.2) in the standard trial, 794.8 (69.4) (95% CI 754.8–834.9) in the protein-rich trial, 832.4 (96.4) (95% CI 776.7–888.1) in the fat-rich trial, and 910.0 (113.2) (95% CI 844.4–975.2) in the carbohydrate-rich trial. On comparing the sum of the AUC for 4 h after lunch and dinner, the carbohydrate-rich trial had significantly higher values than the standard, protein-rich, and fat-rich trials. (P = 0.048, P = 0.002, P = 0.019 vs carbohydrate-rich trial) The number of protein-rich trials was significantly lower than that of the standard and fat-rich trials (P = 0.013, P = 0.041 vs protein-rich trial) (Fig. 3H). The peak blood glucose level after dinner was 141.4 (26.6) (95% CI 126.1–156.8) in the standard trial, 134.6 (16.0) (95% CI 125.3–143.8) in the protein-rich trial, 162.4 (31.8) (95% CI 144.1–180.8) in the fat-rich trial, and 146.9 (31.1) (95% CI 128.9–164.9) in the carbohydrate-rich trial The peak blood glucose level after dinner was significantly higher in the fat-rich trial than that in the protein-rich trial (P = 0.017 vs protein-rich trial) (Fig. 3I).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Postprandial blood glucose levels for lunch and dinner in the balance trial. All data are presented as the mean (standard deviation). Study protocol for the balance trial (A). Glucose concentration (B), area under the curve (AUC) (C), and peak blood glucose levels (D) for 4 h after lunch. Glucose concentration (E) for 4 h after dinner. Incremental AUC (iAUC) (F) and AUC (G) for 2 and 3 h after dinner. The sum of AUC for 4 h after lunch and dinner for each trial in the balance trial (H). Peak blood glucose levels at 4 h after dinner (I). Figure 3C, D, and H used the Friedman test. F, G, and I used One-way ANOVA.

Relationship between starvation time and blood glucose levels

Correlation between the difference in starvation time from breakfast to lunch and blood glucose levels

In the energy trial, no correlation was found between the starvation time from breakfast to lunch (95% CI 4.5–5.4 h, 4.1–5.3 h and 4.4–6.2 h in the low-, standard- and high-energy lunch trial) and the AUC for 4 h after lunch (P = 0.949, P = 0.895, P = 0.637) (Fig. 4A, B, and C). No correlation was found between the starvation time from breakfast to lunch (95% CI 4.6–5.5 h, 4.7–5.9 h, 4.6–5.5 h and 4.4–5.4 h in the standard, protein-rich, fat-rich and carbohydrate-rich trial) and the AUC 4 h after lunch in the balance trial (P = 0.770, P = 0.449, P = 0.953, P = 0.191) (Fig. 4D, E, F, and G).

Fig. 4
figure 4

Correlations between starvation time from breakfast to lunch and postprandial blood glucose levels for lunch. Correlation between starvation time from breakfast to lunch and iAUC for 2 h after lunch in the low- (A), standard- (B), and high- (C) energy trials. Correlations between starvation time from breakfast to lunch and iAUC 2 h after lunch in the standard (D), protein-rich (E), fat-rich (F), and carbohydrate-rich (G) trials.

Correlation between the difference in starvation time from lunch to dinner and blood glucose levels

In the energy trial, on examining the correlation between the starvation time from lunch to dinner (95% CI 10.1–11.5, 5.2–6.2 h, 5.4–6.3 h and 5.1–6.1 h in the no-, low-, standard- and high-energy lunch trial) and the iAUC for 2 h after dinner, a correlation was found only in the high-lunch trial (P < 0.01, r = 0.799) (Fig. 5D). In the balance trial, on examining the correlation between the starvation time from lunch to dinner (95% CI 5.3–6.1 h, 5.3–6.4 h, 5.2–6.1 h and 5.5–6.7 h in the standard, protein-rich, fat-rich and carbohydrate-rich trial) and the iAUC for 2 h after dinner, a correlation was found only in the fat-rich trial (P < 0.01, r = 0.735) (Fig. 5H).

Fig. 5
figure 5

Correlations between starvation time from lunch to dinner and postprandial blood glucose levels for dinner. Correlation between starvation time from lunch to dinner and iAUC for 2 h after lunch in the no (A), low- (B), standard- (C), and high-energy trials (D). Correlations between starvation time from lunch to dinner and iAUC 2 h after dinner in the standard (E), protein-rich (F), fat-rich (G), and carbohydrate-rich (H) trials.

Comparison of the blood glucose levels after dinner on classifying into two groups by mean starvation time in the energy trial

There was a positive correlation between the length of the starvation time from lunch to dinner and blood glucose levels in the high-energy and high-carbohydrate diets (Fig. 5). Therefore, we divided the subjects into two groups: the shorter-starvation group (n = 6, starvation time range 4.2–5.6 h) and the longer-starvation group (n = 7, starvation time range 5.7–7.1 h) by the median mean of starvation time from lunch to dinner in the energy trial.

In the shorter starvation group, the iAUC for 2 h after dinner was 135.0 (65.2) (95% CI 86.7–183.3) in the no-energy lunch trial, 121.8 (43.4) (95% CI 88.4–155.2) in the low-energy lunch trial, 56.6 (35.0) (95% CI 34.4–78.8) in the standard energy trial and 146.9 (31.1) (95% CI 16.5–39.1) in the high-energy trial. The iAUC for 2 h after dinner was significantly lower in the high-intake trial than that in the no-, low-, and standard-lunch trials (P = 0.028, P = 0.028, P = 0.046 vs high-energy lunch trial). Moreover, it was significantly lower in the standard lunch trial than that in the no and low-energy lunch trials (P = 0.028, P = 0.028, P = 0.046 vs standard-energy trial). In the longer starvation group, the iAUC for 2 h after dinner was 112 (54.8) (95% CI 70.9–153.1) in the no-energy lunch trial, 112.8 (66.1) (95% CI 63.4–162.1) in the low-energy lunch trial, 86.5 (44.2) (95% CI 60.6–112.4) in the standard energy trial, and 54.1 (39.0) (95% CI 35.3–72.9) in the high-energy lunch trial.The iAUC for 2 h after dinner was significantly lower in the high-energy lunch trial than those in the no, low-, or standard-energy lunch trials (P = 0.018, P = 0.018, P = 0.043 vs high-energy lunch trial). Comparing the longer-starvation group with the shorter-starvation group, the standard-energy lunch trial in the shorter starvation group showed lower blood glucose levels after dinner (Fig. 6A, B). We examined the physical characteristics of the shorter- and longer-starvation groups. The starvation times were 5.3 (0.5) and 6.3 (0.1) h in the shorter and longer starvation group. The longer starvation group had significantly longer starvation times (P = 0.000) (Additional file 3: Fig. S3A). In addition, the iAUC for 2 h after dinner in the standard trial was 50.0 (23.8) (95% CI 35.0–65.0) and 78.6 (34.5) (95% CI 57.9–99.3) mg/dl*2 h in the shorter and longer starvation group. Comparing the iAUCs for 2 h blood glucose levels after dinner in the standard-lunch trial, the longer-starvation group showed significantly higher values than did the shorter starvation group (P = 0.024) (Additional file 3: Fig. S3B). There were no significant differences in BMI or daily energy intake between groups (P = 0.587, P = 0.723) (Additional file 3: Fig. S3C, D). The step counts were 9786.6 (3786.3) (95% CI 7405.2–12,168.1) and 9653.6 (1289.2) (95% CI 7064.3–12,242.9) steps/day in the shorter and longer starvation group. The MVPA was 99.2 (31.2) (95% CI 79.5–118.8) and 92.4 (37.4) (95% CI 69.7–115.0) min/day in the shorter and longer starvation group. Physical activity levels, such as step counts and MVPA, tended to be higher in the shorter-starvation group than those in the longer-starvation group (P = 0.645, P = 0.245) (Additional file 3: Fig. S3E, F). The daily intake of dietary fiber was 13.7 (3.0) (95% CI 11.8–15.6) and 13.1 (3.0) (95% CI 11.3–14.9) g in the shorter and longer starvation group. The MEQ score was 64.5 (11.7) (95% CI 57.2–71.9) and 61.6 (7.3) (95% CI 57.2–66.0) in the shorter and longer starvation group. The shorter-starvation group tended to consume more dietary fiber and had higher MEQ scores than the longer-starvation group (P = 0.628, P = 0.461) (Additional file 3: Fig. S3G, H).

Fig. 6
figure 6

Comparisons of the blood glucose levels after dinner on classifying into two groups by mean starvation time in the energy and balance trials All the data are presented as the mean (standard deviation). iAUC for 2 h after dinner for each trial for the shorter-starvation (A) and longer-starvation (B) groups in the energy trial. iAUC for 2 h after dinner for each trial for the shorter-starvation (C) and longer-starvation (D) groups in the balance trial. Fig A and B used the Friedman test. C used One-way ANOVA.

Comparison of the blood glucose levels after dinner on classifying into two groups by mean starvation time in the balance trial

We divided the patients into two groups: the shorter starvation group (n = 7, starvation time range 4.4–5.9 h) and the longer starvation group (n = 7, starvation time range 5.9–7.3 h) based on the median mean of starvation time from lunch to dinner in the balance trial.

In the shorter starvation group, the iAUC for 2 h after dinner was 44.3 (27.9) (95% CI 18.5–70.1) in the standard trial, 31.3 (24.4) (95% CI 8.8–53.9) in the protein-rich trial, 99.3 (46.5) (95% CI 56.2–142.3) in the fat-rich trial, and 40.3 (34.7) (95% CI 8.2–72.3) in the carbohydrate-rich trial. The iAUC for 2 h after dinner was significantly higher in the fat-rich trial than those in the standard and carbohydrate-rich trials (P = 0.013, P = 0.015 vs fat-rich trial) (Fig. 6C). However, in the longer-starvation group, the iAUC for 2 h after dinner was 70.7 (43.0) (95% CI 5.9–186.3) in the standard trial, 67.5 (18.7) (95% CI50.2–84.7) in the protein-rich trial, 85.6 (37.9) (95% CI50.5–120.7) in fat-rich trial and 81.2 (40.0) (95% CI 44.1–118.2) in the carbohydrate-rich trial. There were no significant differences between the trials (P = 0.224) (Fig. 6D).

Relationship between the percentage of energy intake during the usual lunch and postprandial blood glucose levels after dinner during the trial

The energy intake and percentage of energy intake of participants in the energy trial are shown in Table 3A, B. The relationship between the 2 h after dinner iAUC and peak blood glucose level for each trial in the energy trial and the usual percentage of energy intake was examined and found to have no significant correlation (P = 0.322, P = 0.197 in usual energy intake, P = 0.514, P = 0.398 in percentage of protein intake, P = 0.456, P = 0.394 in percentage of fat intake, P = 0.325, P = 0.321 in percentage of carbohydrate intake) (Additional file 4: Fig. S4).

Table 3 Energy intake and percentage of energy intake during daily life for participants

The energy intake and percentage of energy intake of the participants in the balance trial are shown in Tables 3C and D. The relationship between the 2 h after dinner iAUC and peak blood glucose level for each trial in the balance trial and the percentage of usual lunch intake was examined. The results showed no significant correlation between the percentage of fat and carbohydrate intake during the usual lunch and postprandial blood glucose levels after dinner. However, there was a positive correlation between the usual percentage of protein intake at lunch and the iAUC at 2 h after dinner (P = 0.088, r = 0.473) and a significant positive correlation with the peak blood glucose level at dinner (P = 0.041, r = 0.551) (Additional file 5: Fig. S5A, D).

Discussion

In the first part of the current study, we found that lunch skipping and/or low-energy lunch, such as 200 kcal, caused a higher increase in postprandial tissue glucose at dinner time. It is well known that breakfast skipping provides an increase in postprandial blood glucose levels at lunchtime in comparison with non-skippers [30]. Our present results supplement this result with information on the risk of lunch skipping and/or small lunch food on the high increase in postprandial glucose, or the so-called blood glucose spike. As per the current understanding of the risk of diabetes, small rather than large daily changes in blood glucose levels between minimum and maximum better offer protection against cardiovascular diseases [33]. Therefore, skipping meals may be harmful, but more evidence is needed. In 2015, a nationwide cross-sectional study in Iran (14,286 students aged 7–18) revealed that the frequencies of breakfast, lunch, and dinner skipping were 13.85, 6.8%, and 7.5%, respectively [10]. The National Nutrition Survey in Japan (NNSJ) in 2019 reported that the frequencies of breakfast, lunch, and dinner skipping were 12.1%, 4.0%, and 1.0%, respectively [34]. These data suggest a risk of lunch skipping like that of breakfast skipping. To understand this scenario, we should determine whether breakfast consumption after skipping dinner causes a greater increase in blood glucose levels.

Starvation with time-restricted feeding (TRF) is now a popular way to reduce obesity, risk of cardiovascular disease, abnormalities in lipid metabolism, and diabetes [35, 36]. Men with prediabetes were randomized to TRF (6 h feeding period, with dinner before 3 p.m.) or a control schedule (12 h feeding period) for 5 weeks and were later crossed over to the other schedule. TRF improves insulin sensitivity, β-cell responsiveness, blood pressure, oxidative stress, and appetite [37]. TRF (10 h feeding period) has been reported to have anti-obesity effects in general people and people with obesity, to promote the recovery of abnormal lipid metabolism, and reduce hypertension in people with diabetes [38]. Interestingly, the delayed eating schedule (food intake limit 1200 h–2300 h) for 8 weeks did not cause an anti-obesity (BMI reduction) effect compared to the daytime eating schedule (food intake limit of 0800 h–1900 h) [39]. Thus, the timing and duration of starvation are key factors for the success of anti-obesity treatment. In our crossover intervention experiments, a 10 h fasting time was set after dinner until the next day's breakfast or after breakfast until dinner. Breakfast consumption provided a lower tissue glucose increase than dinner, even though the same meal was consumed at breakfast and dinner [40]. Our recent findings support the risk of daytime starvation on the control of tissue glucose increase and body weight increase, rather than nighttime starvation.

When the combined lunchtime 4 h AUC of glucose and dinner time 4 h AUC of glucose were analyzed, the total glucose AUC was higher in the standard lunch trial than those in the non-, low-, and high-energy lunch trials (Fig. 2E), suggesting that a slightly lower amount of energy than standard lunch (700 kcal) may be required for glucose level control throughout the day.

In the second part of the current experiment, we found that a high-fat lunch provided a higher increase in tissue glucose at dinner than a standard lunch meal, even if participants took the middle lunch size, such as 700 kcal for men and 526 kcal for women. At lunchtime, a high-fat meal provided a significantly lower level of tissue glucose increase compared with a standard PFC-balanced meal. It was reported that the intake of breakfast containing high fat content provided an increase in tissue glucose at lunchtime compared with a standard PFC-balanced breakfast [41], supporting our current result. Although a high-fat meal does not increase tissue glucose levels at lunchtime, attention should be paid to the high increase in tissue glucose levels at dinner time. The combined data of lunch glucose and dinner glucose AUC values were lower than those in the high-carbohydrate trial. The increase in free fatty acids (FFA) in the blood may be increased by high-fat food, and this increased FFA may reduce insulin sensitivity [42].

In the case of high-carbohydrate meals at lunch, glucose levels at lunchtime were strongly increased, but glucose levels at dinner were similar to those observed in the standard lunch trial. On combining lunchtime and dinner time 4 h glucose AUC values, the carbohydrate group showed significantly higher levels than the standard, high protein, and high-fat trials, suggesting that carbohydrate-rich lunches confer a greater risk of high glucose levels throughout the day. In the case of protein-rich meals at lunch, glucose levels tended to reduce the glucose AUC at lunchtime. A review reported that whey proteins, rich in branched-chain amino acids (BCAAs) such as leucine, isoleucine, valine, and lysine may decrease postprandial glucose responses and stimulate insulin secretion in healthy individuals and patients with type 2 diabetes [43, 44]. In addition, protein-rich foods increase GLP-1, and GLP-1 increase may be involved in the second-meal effect [43]. As protein-rich food itself has a weak effect on tissue glucose and protein-rich food provides the second-meal effect, these two effects may be related to low levels of glucose throughout the day.

In this experiment, starvation periods between breakfast and lunch and between lunch and dinner were dependent on the participants’ lifestyle, because we asked them to maintain their mealtime patterns throughout the experiments. Therefore, there were 4–6 h differences between breakfast and lunch, and 4–7 h differences between lunch and dinner. There was no association between the starvation period and standard meal-induced tissue glucose level at lunch in experiments 1 (energy difference of lunch) and 2 (nutrient balance of lunch). The current observation suggests that a balanced and calculated amount of energy meal at breakfast can control the next meal-induced tissue glucose level equally at early or late lunchtime. In future experiments, we aim to investigate whether the composition and/or energy quantity of breakfast has a similar effect on lunchtime. Similarly, we checked whether lunch with different energy quantities or unbalanced PFC percentages affected standard meal-induced tissue glucose levels at early or late dinner time. There was no association between tissue glucose level and starvation period at dinner in the 0 kcal, 200 kcal, and 700 kcal lunch trials, but there was a significant positive correlation between the starvation period and glucose levels in the 1200 kcal trial. There was no association between tissue glucose level and starvation period at dinner in the PFC-balanced, protein-rich, and fat-rich lunch trials; however, there was a significant positive correlation between the starvation period and glucose levels in the carbohydrate-rich lunch trial. Longer starvation with meal skipping augments postprandial glucose increase in the next meal [27,28,29,30, 45]. Although the mechanism of these results is currently unknown, insulin secretion and/or sensitivity [28, 30], GLP-1 secretion [44], and other unknown factors may be involved in this second-meal effect.

As starvation between lunch and dinner may affect glucose levels at dinner, we divided the participants into two groups (shorter starvation group and longer starvation group) by the median value of the starvation period. The shorter starvation group showed lower tissue glucose levels at dinner in the standard lunch trial compared with the longer starvation group. In general, later dinners are known to produce higher glucose levels [25]; therefore, higher levels of glucose at dinner were observed in the longer-starvation group in the standard and high-energy lunch trials. In the next experiment, we examined the characteristics of the two shorter and longer groups. The shorter starvation group showed higher physical activity [21], higher dietary fiber intake [19, 20], and a tendency towards morningness in the MEQ score [21]. These tendencies are related to a reduction in blood glucose levels. The shorter starvation group provided higher tissue glucose levels at dinner time in the high-fat lunch trial than the longer starvation group. Furthermore, FFAs block insulin action [42, 46]. FFA production by high-fat food may be high at an early dinner time and may interfere with the insulin effect, but may not maintain high levels at a later dinner time.

In the current experiment, we examined whether the intervention of energy and PFC balance differences at lunch affected tissue glucose levels at dinner time. Therefore, the daily intake of lunch energy and PFC balance assessed by FFQ may be related to lunch intervention on tissue glucose levels at dinner. There was no association between daily lunch energy (kcal), lunch PFC balance (%), and glucose levels at dinner in the standard-lunch trial. Further, we investigated whether the daily lunch protein, fat, and carbohydrate ratio (%) affect glucose levels at dinner time in the protein-rich, fat-rich, and carbohydrate-rich lunch trials, respectively. There was a strong positive association between the daily protein ratio (%) and maximum values (P = 0.041, r = 0.551) and 2-h iAUC (P = 0.088, r = 0.473) of tissue glucose levels at dinner. As protein-rich lunches led to low levels of tissue glucose at dinner due to the second meal effect, we hypothesized a negative association between daily lunch protein quantity and tissue glucose levels at dinner. Although the possible mechanism of the present results is unclear, the daily intake of protein-rich lunches may cause the downregulation of mechanisms of the second-meal effect.

Physical activity may affect the control of tissue glucose changes, and we examined the association between standard dinner-induced glucose increase and physical activity. There was no association between these two factors, suggesting that physical activity levels and tissue glucose changes did not influence the present study.

Limitation of experiments

Our study has some limitations. First, the participants were healthy young adults. Our results may not be applicable to other subjects, such as healthy older men and women, or patients with diabetes. However, only a few studies have focused on lunch. Therefore, the results of this study are important as they may lead to future research. Second, the effects of differences in dietary intake on non-trial days on blood glucose fluctuations cannot be ruled out. A test meal was served on each trial day. However, the meals on other days were not controlled and may have influenced blood glucose fluctuations. On the other hand, the participants were instructed to maintain their normal lifestyle throughout the experiment. Therefore, the effect of differences in food intake on non-trial days on blood glucose levels is considered weak. Prospects for future research include study protocols for controlling diet during the entire study period with less influence of the usual diet, which may lead to more detailed clarification.

Availability of data and materials

All data generated or analyzed during this study are included in this published article and its additional files.

Abbreviations

AUC:

Area under the curve

BCAAs:

Branched-chain amino acids

BIA:

Bioelectrical impedance analysis

BMI:

Body mass index

CGM:

Continuous glucose monitoring

FFA:

Free fatty acids

FFQ:

Food frequency questionnaire

HPI:

Healthy purchase index

iAUC:

Incremental area under the curve

MEQ:

Morningness–eveningness questionnaire

MVPA:

Moderate-to-vigorous physical activity

NNSJ:

National nutrition survey in Japan

PFC Balance:

Protein, fat, and carbohydrate balance

TRF:

Time-restricted feeding

References

  1. Fong M, Caterson ID, Madigan CD. Are large dinners associated with excess weight, and does eating a smaller dinner achieve greater weight loss? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Br J Nutr. 2017;118:616–28.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Zerón-Rugerio MF, Hernáez Á, Porras-Loaiza AP, Cambras T, Izquierdo-Pulido M. Eating jet lag: a marker of the variability in meal timing and its association with body mass index. Nutrients. 2019;11:2980.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. Schwingshackl L, Nitschke K, Zähringer J, Bischoff K, Lohner S, Torbahn G, Schlesinger S, Schmucker C, Meerpohl JJ. Impact of meal frequency on anthropometric outcomes: a systematic review and network meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Adv Nutr. 2020;11:1108–22.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Ma Y, Bertone ER, Stanek EJ 3rd, Reed GW, Hebert JR, Cohen NL, Merriam PA, Ockene IS. Association between eating patterns and obesity in a free-living US adult population. Am J Epidemiol. 2003;158:85–92.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Smith KJ, Gall SL, McNaughton SA, Blizzard L, Dwyer T, Venn AJ. Skipping breakfast: longitudinal associations with cardiometabolic risk factors in the childhood determinants of adult health study. Am J Clin Nutr. 2010;92:1316–25.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Adolphus K, Lawton CL, Champ CL, Dye L. The effects of breakfast and breakfast composition on cognition in children and adolescents: a systematic review. Adv Nutr. 2016;7:590s–612s.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Imamura M, Sasaki H, Shinto T, Tahara Y, Makino S, Kuwahara M, Tada A, Abe N, Michie M, Shibata S. Association between Na, K, and lipid intake in each meal and blood pressure. Front Nutr. 2022;9: 853118.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  8. McCurley JL, Levy DE, Dashti HS, Gelsomin E, Anderson E, Sonnenblick R, Rimm EB, Thorndike AN. Association of employees’ meal skipping patterns with workplace food purchases, dietary quality, and cardiometabolic risk: a secondary analysis from the choosewell 365 trial. J Acad Nutr Diet. 2022;122:110-120.e112.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Chin YS, Woon FC, Chan YM. The impact of movement control order during the COVID-19 pandemic on lifestyle behaviours and body weight changes: findings from the MyNutriLifeCOVID-19 online survey. PLoS ONE. 2022;17: e0262332.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Horn C, Laupsa-Borge J, Andersen AIO, Dyer L, Revheim I, Leikanger T, Næsheim NT, Storås I, Johannessen KK, Mellgren G, et al. Meal patterns associated with energy intake in people with obesity. Br J Nutr. 2021;128:1–11.

    Google Scholar 

  11. Nilsson A, Ostman E, Preston T, Björck I. Effects of GI vs content of cereal fibre of the evening meal on glucose tolerance at a subsequent standardized breakfast. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2008;62:712–20.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Rahat-Rozenbloom S, Fernandes J, Cheng J, Gloor GB, Wolever TM. The acute effects of inulin and resistant starch on postprandial serum short-chain fatty acids and second-meal glycemic response in lean and overweight humans. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2017;71:227–33.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Brighenti F, Benini L, Del Rio D, Casiraghi C, Pellegrini N, Scazzina F, Jenkins DJ, Vantini I. Colonic fermentation of indigestible carbohydrates contributes to the second-meal effect. Am J Clin Nutr. 2006;83:817–22.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Jenkins DJ, Wolever TM, Nineham R, Sarson DL, Bloom SR, Ahern J, Alberti KG, Hockaday TD. Improved glucose tolerance four hours after taking guar with glucose. Diabetologia. 1980;19:21–4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Liljeberg H, Björck I. Effects of a low-glycaemic index spaghetti meal on glucose tolerance and lipaemia at a subsequent meal in healthy subjects. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2000;54:24–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Chen MJ, Jovanovic A, Taylor R. Utilizing the second-meal effect in type 2 diabetes: practical use of a soya-yogurt snack. Diabetes Care. 2010;33:2552–4.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Meng H, Matthan NR, Ausman LM, Lichtenstein AH. Effect of prior meal macronutrient composition on postprandial glycemic responses and glycemic index and glycemic load value determinations. Am J Clin Nutr. 2017;106:1246–56.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Fletcher JA, Perfield II, Jw TJ, Rector RS. The second meal effect and its influence on glycemia. J Nutr Disorders Ther. 2012;2:1–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Kim HK, Chijiki H, Nanba T, Ozaki M, Sasaki H, Takahashi M, Shibata S. Ingestion of Helianthus tuberosus at breakfast rather than at dinner is more effective for suppressing glucose levels and improving the intestinal microbiota in older adults. Nutrients. 2020;12:3035.

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. Kim HK, Nanba T, Ozaki M, Chijiki H, Takahashi M, Fukazawa M, Okubo J, Shibata S. Effect of the intake of a snack containing dietary fiber on postprandial glucose levels. Foods. 2020;9:1500.

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  21. Kuwahara M, Kim HK, Ozaki M, Nanba T, Chijiki H, Fukazawa M, Okubo J, Mineshita Y, Takahashi M, Shibata S. Consumption of biscuits with a beverage of mulberry or barley leaves in the afternoon prevents dinner-induced high, but not low, increases in blood glucose among young adults. Nutrients. 2020;12:1580.

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  22. Imai S, Kajiyama S, Hashimoto Y, Nitta A, Miyawaki T, Matsumoto S, Ozasa N, Tanaka M, Kajiyama S, Fukui M. Consuming snacks mid-afternoon compared with just after lunch improves mean amplitude of glycaemic excursions in patients with type 2 diabetes: a randomized crossover clinical trial. Diabetes Metab. 2018;44:482–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Van Cauter E, Shapiro ET, Tillil H, Polonsky KS. Circadian modulation of glucose and insulin responses to meals: relationship to cortisol rhythm. Am J Physiol. 1992;262:E467-475.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Service FJ, Hall LD, Westland RE, O’Brien PC, Go VL, Haymond MW, Rizza RA. Effects of size, time of day and sequence of meal ingestion on carbohydrate tolerance in normal subjects. Diabetologia. 1983;25:316–21.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Takahashi M, Ozaki M, Miyashita M, Fukazawa M, Nakaoka T, Wakisaka T, Matsui Y, Hibi M, Osaki N, Shibata S. Effects of timing of acute catechin-rich green tea ingestion on postprandial glucose metabolism in healthy men. J Nutr Biochem. 2019;73: 108221.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Salle L, Millotte C, Vergonjeanne M, Druet-Cabanac M, Perrier M, Gravelat M, Dupont-Cuisinier M, Le Flahec C, Desport JC, Philippe F, Jésus P. Prevalence of tachyphagia at lunch and associated factors in a population of workers. Eat Weight Disord. 2022;27:1569–74.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Jovanovic A, Gerrard J, Taylor R. The second-meal phenomenon in type 2 diabetes. Diabetes Care. 2009;32:1199–201.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Lee SH, Tura A, Mari A, Ko SH, Kwon HS, Song KH, Yoon KH, Lee KW, Ahn YB. Potentiation of the early-phase insulin response by a prior meal contributes to the second-meal phenomenon in type 2 diabetes. Am J Physiol Endocrinol Metab. 2011;301:E984-990.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Chowdhury EA, Richardson JD, Tsintzas K, Thompson D, Betts JA. Carbohydrate-rich breakfast attenuates glycaemic, insulinaemic and ghrelin response to ad libitum lunch relative to morning fasting in lean adults. Br J Nutr. 2015;114:98–107.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. Jakubowicz D, Wainstein J, Ahren B, Landau Z, Bar-Dayan Y, Froy O. Fasting until noon triggers increased postprandial hyperglycemia and impaired insulin response after lunch and dinner in individuals with type 2 diabetes: a randomized clinical trial. Diabetes Care. 2015;38:1820–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Horne JA, Ostberg O. A self-assessment questionnaire to determine morningness-eveningness in human circadian rhythms. Int J Chronobiol. 1976;4:97–110.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Wakai K. A review of food frequency questionnaires developed and validated in Japan. J Epidemiol. 2009;19:1–11.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. Akasaka T, Sueta D, Tabata N, Takashio S, Yamamoto E, Izumiya Y, Tsujita K, Kojima S, Kaikita K, Matsui K, Hokimoto S. Effects of the mean amplitude of glycemic excursions and vascular endothelial dysfunction on cardiovascular events in nondiabetic patients with coronary artery disease. J Am Heart Assoc. 2017;6:e004841.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. The national health and nutrition survey in Japan [https://www.mhlw.go.jp/stf/seisakunitsuite/bunya/kenkou_iryou/kenkou/eiyou/r1-houkoku_00002.html]

  35. Schuppelius B, Peters B, Ottawa A, Pivovarova-Ramich O. Time restricted eating: a dietary strategy to prevent and treat metabolic disturbances. Front Endocrinol (Lausanne). 2021;12: 683140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Tacad DKM, Tovar AP, Richardson CE, Horn WF, Krishnan GP, Keim NL, Krishnan S. Satiety associated with calorie restriction and time-restricted feeding: peripheral hormones. Adv Nutr. 2022. https://doi.org/10.1093/advances/nmac011.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Sutton EF, Beyl R, Early KS, Cefalu WT, Ravussin E, Peterson CM. Early time-restricted feeding improves insulin sensitivity, blood pressure, and oxidative stress even without weight loss in men with prediabetes. Cell Metab. 2018;27:1212-1221.e1213.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  38. Wilkinson MJ, Manoogian ENC, Zadourian A, Lo H, Fakhouri S, Shoghi A, Wang X, Fleischer JG, Navlakha S, Panda S, Taub PR. Ten-hour time-restricted eating reduces weight, blood pressure, and atherogenic lipids in patients with metabolic syndrome. Cell Metab. 2020;31:92-104.e105.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Allison KC, Hopkins CM, Ruggieri M, Spaeth AM, Ahima RS, Zhang Z, Taylor DM, Goel N. Prolonged, controlled daytime versus delayed eating impacts weight and metabolism. Curr Biol. 2021;31:650-657.e653.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Mineshita Y, Sasaki H, Kim H-K, Shibata S. Relationship between fasting and postprandial glucose levels and the gut microbiota, Research Square 2021, PREPRINT (Version 1)

  41. Ando T, Nakae S, Usui C, Yoshimura E, Nishi N, Takimoto H, Tanaka S. Effect of diurnal variations in the carbohydrate and fat composition of meals on postprandial glycemic response in healthy adults: a novel insight for the second-meal phenomenon. Am J Clin Nutr. 2018;108:332–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Roden M, Price TB, Perseghin G, Petersen KF, Rothman DL, Cline GW, Shulman GI. Mechanism of free fatty acid-induced insulin resistance in humans. J Clin Invest. 1996;97:2859–65.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  43. Chartrand D, Da Silva MS, Julien P, Rudkowska I. Influence of amino acids in dairy products on glucose homeostasis: the clinical evidence. Can J Diabetes. 2017;41:329–37.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Pasin G, Comerford KB. Dairy foods and dairy proteins in the management of type 2 diabetes: a systematic review of the clinical evidence. Adv Nutr. 2015;6:245–59.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  45. Jovanovic A, Leverton E, Solanky B, Ravikumar B, Snaar JE, Morris PG, Taylor R. The second-meal phenomenon is associated with enhanced muscle glycogen storage in humans. Clin Sci (Lond). 2009;117:119–27.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  46. Dresner A, Laurent D, Marcucci M, Griffin ME, Dufour S, Cline GW, Slezak LA, Andersen DK, Hundal RS, Rothman DL, et al. Effects of free fatty acids on glucose transport and IRS-1-associated phosphatidylinositol 3-kinase activity. J Clin Invest. 1999;103:253–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank all the investigators and volunteers who participated in this study.

Funding

This work was supported by the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (KAKENHI Grant Number 19H01089 to S.S.) and JST-Mirai Program Grant Number JMPJM120D5, Japan.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

The authors’ responsibilities were as follows: H-KK, MK, and SS conceptualization; MK and AF data curation; H-KK, AF, and MK formal analysis; SS funding acquisition; H-KK, YM, and MS investigation; H-KK, MK, and SS methodology; H-KK and SS project administration; SS resources; SS supervision; H-KK and MK visualization; H-KK, MK, and SS writing—original draft; and, TN and SS writing—review and editing. All the authors have read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Shigenobu Shibata.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Informed consent was obtained from all participants after explaining the experiment details to them (i.e. purpose, methods). The study protocol conforms to the Helsinki Declaration and was approved by the Ethics Committee for Humans at Waseda University (approval numbers: 2020–371).

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no financial or non-financial competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1

. Fig. S1: Relationship between physical activity levels during each trial period and iAUC for 2 h after dinner. Correlation between physical activity levels during the energy trial period and iAUC for 2 h after dinner in the standard trial (A, B). Correlation between physical activity levels during the balance trial period and iAUC for 2 h after dinner in the standard trial (C and D). MVPA: moderate-to-vigorous physical activity.

Additional file 2

. Fig. S2: Glucose excursion for 24 h in the energy trial group (A), and the balance trial group (B). Significant differences between trials after lunch at each 15-minute time interval in the energy trial group. *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01 (between no- and low-energy lunch trial), †P < 0.05, ††P < 0.01, †††P < 0.001 (between no- and standard-energy lunch trial), ‡P < 0.05,‡‡P < 0.01, ‡‡‡P < 0.001 (between no- and high-energy lunch trial), &P < 0.05, &&P < 0.01, &&&P < 0.001 (between low- and standard-energy lunch trial), $P < 0.05, $$P < 0.01, $$$P < 0.001 (between low- and high-energy lunch trial), ##P < 0.01, ###P < 0.001 (between standard- and high-energy lunch trial) (Two-way ANOVA) (C). Significant differences between trials after dinner at each 15-minute time interval in the energy trial group. ††P < 0.01, †††P < 0.001 (between standard and fat-rich trial), ‡P < 0.05 (between standard and carbohydrate-rich trial), $P < 0.05, $$P < 0.005, $$$P < 0.001 (between protein-rich and carbohydrate-rich trial), #P < 0.05 (between fat-rich and carbohydrate-rich trial) (D). Significant differences between trials after lunch at each 15-minute time interval in the balance trial group. †P < 0.05 (between no- and standard-energy lunch trial), ‡P < 0.05, ‡‡P < 0.01, ‡‡‡P < 0.001 (between no- and high-energy lunch trial), $P < 0.05, $$P < 0.01, $$$P < 0.001 (between low- and high-energy lunch trial), #P < 0.01, ##P < 0.005, ###P < 0.001 (between standard- and high-energy lunch trial) (Two-way ANOVA) (E). Significant differences between trials at each 15-minute time interval in the balance trial group †P < 0.05 (between standard and fat-rich trial), $P < 0.05, $$P < 0.01 (between low- and high-energy lunch trial) (Two-way ANOVA) (F).

Additional file 3

. Fig. S3: Characteristics after classifying into two groups by mean starvation time. Starvation time (A), iAUC for 2 h after dinner (B), body mass index (C), energy intake (D), step counts (E), moderate-to-vigorous physical activity (F), intake of dietary fiber (G), and MEQ (H). MEQ; Morningness–Eveningness Questionnaire. A t-test was used to test for differences between groups.

Additional file 4

. Fig. S4: Association between lunch energy intake during daily life and after-dinner blood glucose levels in the energy trial. Relationship between intake (A), percentage of protein (B), percentage of fat (C), percentage of carbohydrate (D) during daily life for lunch, and iAUC for 2 h after dinner in the standard trial. Relationship between intake (E), percentage of protein (F), percentage of fat (G), percentage of carbohydrates (H) during daily life for lunch, and peak blood glucose levels after dinner in the standard trial.

Additional file 5

. Fig. S5: Association between lunch energy intake during daily life and after-dinner blood glucose levels in the balance trial. Relationship between percentage of protein during daily life for lunch and iAUC for 2 h after dinner in the protein-rich trial (A). Relationship between the percentage of fat during daily life for lunch and iAUC for 2 h after dinner in the fat-rich trial (B). Relationship between percentage of carbohydrates during daily life for lunch and iAUC for 2 h after dinner in the carbohydrate-rich trial (C). Relationship between percentage of protein (D), fat (E), and carbohydrate (F) during daily life for lunch and peak blood glucose levels after dinner in the standard trial.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Kuwahara, M., Kim, HK., Furutani, A. et al. Effect of lunch with different calorie and nutrient balances on dinner-induced postprandial glucose variability. Nutr Metab (Lond) 19, 65 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12986-022-00704-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12986-022-00704-1

Keywords